What research method does the Strange Situation use?

  1. Attachment
  2. Strange Situation

Mary Ainsworth

The Strange Situation | Attachment Styles

By, updated 2018


The strange situation is a standardized procedure devised by Mary Ainsworth in the 1970s to observe attachment security in children within the context of caregiver relationships. It applies to infants between the age of nine and 18 months.

The procedure involves series of eight episodes lasting approximately 3 minutes each, whereby a mother, child and stranger are introduced, separated and reunited.

John Bowlby (1969) believed that attachment was an all or nothing process. However, research has shown that there are individual differences in attachment quality. Indeed, one of the primary paradigms in attachment theory is that of the security of an individual’s attachment (Ainsworth & Bell, 1970).

Much research in psychology has focused on how forms of attachment differ among infants. For example, Schaffer and Emerson (1964) discovered what appeared to be innate differences in sociability in babies; some babies preferred cuddling more than others, from very early on, before much interaction had occurred to cause such differences.

It’s easy enough to know when you are attached to someone because you know how you feel when you are apart from that person, and, being an adult, you can put your feelings into words and describe how it feels.

However, most attachment research is carried out using infants and young children, so psychologists have to devise subtle ways of researching attachment styles, usually involving the observational method.

Psychologist Mary Ainsworth devised an assessment technique called the Strange Situation Classification (SSC) in order to investigate how attachments might vary between children.

The Strange Situation was devised by Ainsworth and Wittig (1969) and was based on Ainsworth’s previous Uganda (1967) and later Baltimore studies (Ainsworth et al., 1971, 1978).


Mary Ainsworth's (1971, 1978) observational study of individual differences in attachment is described below.

Strange Situation Procedure

The security of attachment in one- to two-year-olds were investigated using the strange situation paradigm, in order to determine the nature of attachment behaviors and styles of attachment.

Ainsworth developed an experimental procedure in order to observe the variety of attachment forms exhibited between mothers and infants.

The experiment is set up in a small room with one way glass so the behavior of the infant can be observed covertly. Infants were aged between 12 and 18 months. The sample comprised of 100 middle-class American families.

The procedure, known as the ‘Strange Situation,’ was conducted by observing the behavior of the infant in a series of eight episodes lasting approximately 3 minutes each:

(1) Mother, baby, and experimenter (lasts less than one minute).

(2) Mother and baby alone.

(3) A stranger joins the mother and infant.

(4) Mother leaves baby and stranger alone.

(5) Mother returns and stranger leaves.

(6) Mother leaves; infant left completely alone.

(7) Stranger returns.

(8) Mother returns and stranger leaves.

Scoring

Scoring

Strange Situation classifications (i.e., attachment styles) are based primarily on four interaction behaviors directed toward the mother in the two reunion episodes (Ep. 5 & Ep. 8).

  1. Proximity and contacting seeking
  2. Contact maintaining
  3. Avoidance of proximity and contact
  4. Resistance to contact and comforting

The observer notes down the behavior displayed during 15-second intervals and scores the behavior for intensity on a scale of 1 to 7.

What research method does the Strange Situation use?

Other behaviors observed included:

  • Exploratory behaviors e.g., moving around the room, playing with toys, looking around the room.
  • Search behaviors, e.g., following mother to the door, banging on the door, orienting to the door, looking at the door, going to mother’s empty chair, looking at mother’s empty chair.
  • Affect Displays negative, e.g., crying, smiling.

Results - Attachment Styles

Ainsworth (1970) identified three main attachment styles, secure (type B), insecure avoidant (type A) and insecure ambivalent/resistant (type C). She concluded that these attachment styles were the result of early interactions with the mother.

A fourth attachment style known as disorganized was later identified (Main, & Solomon, 1990).

SecureResistantAvoidant
Separation Anxiety Distressed when mother leaves Intense distress when the mother leaves No sign of distress when the the mother leaves
Stranger Anxiety Avoidant of stranger when alone, but friendly when the mother is present The infant avoids the stranger - shows fear of the stranger The infant is okay with the stranger and plays normally when the stranger is present
Reunion behavior Positive and happy when mother returns The infant approaches the mother, but resists contact, may even push her away The Infant shows little interest when the mother returns
Other Uses the mother as a safe base to explore their environment The infant cries more and explores less than the other two types The mother and stranger are able to comfort the infant equally well
% of infants 70% 15% 15%

B: Secure Attachment

B: Secure Attachment

Securely attached children comprised the majority of the sample in Ainsworth’s (1971, 1978) studies.

Such children feel confident that the attachment figure will be available to meet their needs. They use the attachment figure as a safe base to explore the environment and seek the attachment figure in times of distress (Main, & Cassidy, 1988).

Securely attached infants are easily soothed by the attachment figure when upset. Infants develop a secure attachment when the caregiver is sensitive to their signals, and responds appropriately to their needs.

According to Bowlby (1980), an individual who has experienced a secure attachment 'is likely to possess a representational model of attachment figures(s) as being available, responsive, and helpful' (Bowlby, 1980, p. 242).


A: Insecure Avoidant

A: Insecure Avoidant

Insecure avoidant children do not orientate to their attachment figure while investigating the environment.

They are very independent of the attachment figure both physically and emotionally (Behrens, Hesse, & Main, 2007).

They do not seek contact with the attachment figure when distressed. Such children are likely to have a caregiver who is insensitive and rejecting of their needs (Ainsworth, 1979).

The attachment figure may withdraw from helping during difficult tasks (Stevenson-Hinde, & Verschueren, 2002) and is often unavailable during times of emotional distress.


C: Insecure Ambivalent / Resistant

C: Insecure Ambivalent

The third attachment style identified by Ainsworth (1970) was insecure ambivalent (also called insecure resistant).

Here children adopt an ambivalent behavioral style towards the attachment figure. The child will commonly exhibit clingy and dependent behavior, but will be rejecting of the attachment figure when they engage in interaction.

The child fails to develop any feelings of security from the attachment figure. Accordingly, they exhibit difficulty moving away from the attachment figure to explore novel surroundings.

When distressed they are difficult to soothe and are not comforted by interaction with the attachment figure. This behavior results from an inconsistent level of response to their needs from the primary caregiver.


Strange Situation Conclusion

Ainsworth (1978) suggested the ‘caregiver sensitivity hypothesis’ as an explanation for different attachment types. Ainsworth's maternal sensitivity hypothesis argues that a child’s attachment style is dependent on the behavior their mother shows towards them.

  • ‘Sensitive’ mothers are responsive to the child's needs and respond to their moods and feelings correctly. Sensitive mothers are more likely to have securely attached children.
  • In contrast, mothers who are less sensitive towards their child, for example, those who respond to the child’s needs incorrectly or who are impatient or ignore the child, are likely to have insecurely attached children.

For example, securely attached infant are associated with sensitive and responsive primary care. Insecure ambivalent attached infants are associated with inconsistent primary care. Sometimes the child’s needs and met, and sometimes they are ignored by the mother / father.

Insecure-avoidant infants are associated with unresponsive primary care. The child comes to believe that communication of needs has no influence on the mother/father.

Ainsworth’s (1971, 1978) findings provided the first empirical evidence for Bowlby’s attachment theory.

For example, securely attached children develop a positive working model of themselves and have mental representations of others as being helpful while viewing themselves as worthy of respect (Jacobsen, & Hoffman, 1997).

Avoidant children think themselves unworthy and unacceptable, caused by a rejecting primary caregiver (Larose, & Bernier, 2001). Ambivalent children have a negative self-image and exaggerate their emotional responses as a way to gain attention (Kobak et al., 1993). 

Accordingly, insecure attachment styles are associated with an increased risk of social and emotional behavioral problems via the internal working model.

What research method does the Strange Situation use?


Theoretical Evaluation

This caregiver sensitivity theory is supported by research from, Wolff and Van Ijzendoorn (1997) who conducted a Meta-analysis (a review) of research into attachment types.

They found that there is a relatively weak correlation of 0.24 between parental sensitivity and attachment type – generally more sensitive parents had securely attached children.

However, in evaluation, critics of this theory argue that the correlation between parental sensitivity and the child’s attachment type is only weak. This suggests that there are other reasons which may better explain why children develop different attachment types and that the maternal sensitivity theory places too much emphasis on the mother.

Focusing just on maternal sensitivity when trying to explain why children have different attachment types is, therefore, a reductionist approach.

An alternative theory proposed by Kagan (1984) suggests that the temperament of the child is actually what leads to the different attachment types. Children with different innate (inborn) temperaments will have different attachment types.

This theory is supported by research from Fox (1989) who found that babies with an ‘Easy’ temperament (those who eat and sleep regularly, and accept new experiences) are likely to develop secure attachments.

Babies with a ‘slow to warm up’ temperament (those who took a while to get used to new experiences) are likely to have insecure-avoidant attachments. Babies with a ‘Difficult’ temperament (those who eat and sleep irregularly and who reject new experiences) are likely to have insecure-ambivalent attachments.

In conclusion, the most complete explanation of why children develop different attachment types would be an interactionist theory. This would argue that a child’s attachment type is a result of a combination of factors – both the child’s innate temperament and their parent’s sensitivity towards their needs.

Belsky and Rovine (1987) propose an interesting interactionist theory to explain the different attachment types. They argue that the child’s attachment type is a result of both the child’s innate temperament and also how the parent responds to them (i.e., the parents’ sensitivity level).

Additionally, the child’s innate temperament may, in fact, influence the way their parent responds to them (i.e, the infants’ temperament influences the parental sensitivity shown to them). To develop a secure attachment, a ‘difficult’ child would need a caregiver who is sensitive and patient for a secure attachment to develop.


Methodological Evaluation

The strange situation classification has been found to have good reliability.  This means that it achieves consistent results.  For example, a study conducted in Germany found 78% of the children were classified in the same way at ages 1 and 6 years (Wartner et al., 1994).

Although, as Melhuish (1993) suggests, the Strange Situation is the most widely used method for assessing infant attachment to a caregiver, Lamb et al. (1985) have criticized it for being highly artificial and therefore lacking ecological validity.

The child is placed in a strange and artificial environment, and the procedure of the mother and stranger entering and leaving the room follows a predetermined script.

Mary Ainsworth concluded that the strange situation could be used to identify the child's type of attachment has been criticized on the grounds that it identifies only the type of attachment to the mother. The child may have a different type of attachment to the father or grandmother, for example (Lamb, 1977). This means that it lacks validity, as it does not measure a general attachment style, but instead an attachment style specific to the mother.

In addition, some research has shown that the same child may show different attachment behaviors on different occasions. Children's attachments may change, perhaps because of changes in the child's circumstances, so a securely attached child may appear insecurely attached if the mother becomes ill or the family circumstances change.

The strange situation has also been criticized on ethical grounds. Because the child is put under stress (separation and stranger anxiety), the study has broken the ethical guideline protection of participants.

However, in its defense, the separation episodes were curtailed prematurely if the child became too stressed. Also, according to Marrone (1998), although the Strange Situation has been criticized for being stressful, it is simulating everyday experiences, as mothers do leave their babies for brief periods of time in different settings and often with unfamiliar people such as babysitters.

Finally, the study's sample is biased - comprising 100 middle-class American families. Therefore, it is difficult to generalize the findings outside of America and to working-class families.

How to reference this article:

How to reference this article:

McLeod, S. A. (2018, August 05). Mary ainsworth. Simply Psychology. www.simplypsychology.org/mary-ainsworth.html

APA Style References

Ainsworth, M. D. (1964). Patterns of attachment behavior shown by the infant in interaction with his mother. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly of behavior and Development, 51-58.

Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1967). Infancy in Uganda: Infant care and the growth of love.

Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1979). Attachment as related to mother-infant interaction. In Advances in the study of behavior (Vol. 9, pp. 1-51). Academic Press.

Ainsworth, M. D. S., & Bell, S. M. (1970). Attachment, exploration, and separation: Illustrated by the behavior of one-year-olds in a strange situation. Child Development, 41, 49-67.

Ainsworth, M. D. S., Bell, S. M., & Stayton, D. J. (1971) Individual differences in strange- situation behavior of one-year-olds. In H. R. Schaffer (Ed.) The origins of human social relations. London and New York: Academic Press. Pp. 17-58.

Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Ainsworth, M. D. S., & Wittig, B. A. (1969). Attachment and exploratory behavior of one-year-olds in a strange situation. In B. M. Foss(Ed. ), Determinants of infant behavior (Vol. 4,pp. 111-136). London: Methuen.

Behrens, K. Y., Hesse, E., & Main, M. (2007). Mothers' attachment status as determined by the Adult Attachment Interview predicts their 6-year-olds' reunion responses: A study conducted in Japan. Developmental Psychology, 43(6), 1553.

Belsky, J., & Rovine, M. (1987). Temperament and attachment security in the strange situation: An empirical rapprochement. Child development, 787-795.

Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment. Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Loss. New York: Basic Books.

Bowlby, J. (1980). Loss: Sadness & depression. Attachment and loss (vol. 3); (International psycho-analytical library no.109). London: Hogarth Press.

Fox, N. A. (1989). Infant temperament and security of attachment: a new look. International Society for behavioral Development, J yviiskylii, Finland.

Jacobsen, T., & Hoffman, V. (1997). Children’s attachment representations: Longitudinal relations to school behavior and academic competency in middle childhood and adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 33, 703-710.

Kagan, J., Reznick, J. S., Clarke, C., Snidman, N., & Garcia-Coll, C. (1984). behavioral inhibition to the unfamiliar. Child development, 2212-2225.

Kobak, R. R., Cole, H. E., Ferenz-Gillies, R., Flemming, W. S., & Gamble, W. (1993). Attachment and emotional regulation during mother-teen problem-solving. A control theory analysis. Child Development, 64, 231-245.

Lamb, M. E. (1977). The development of mother-infant and father-infant attachments in the second year of life. Developmental Psychology, 13, 637-48.

Larose, S., & Bernier, A. (2001). Social support processes: Mediators of attachment state of mind and adjustment in later late adolescence. Attachment and Human Development, 3, 96-120.

Main, M., & Solomon, J. (1990). Procedures for identifying infants as disorganized/disoriented during the Ainsworth Strange Situation. In M.T. Greenberg, D. Cicchetti & E.M. Cummings (Eds.), Attachment in the Preschool Years (pp. 121–160). Chicago, University of Chicago Press.

Marrone, M. (1998). Attachment and interaction. Jessica Kingsley Publishers.

Melhuish, E. C. (1993). A measure of love? An overview of the assessment of attachment. ACPP Review & Newsletter, 15, 269-275.

Schaffer, H. R., & Emerson, P. E. (1964) The development of social attachments in infancy. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 29(3), serial number 94.

Stevenson-Hinde, J., &Attachment in childhood. status: published.&lt;/p&gt;&lt;p&gt;Thompson, R. A., Gardner, W., &amp;amp; Charnov, E. L. (1985). &lt;em&gt;Infant-mother attachment: The origins and developmental significance of individual differences in Strange Situation behavior. &lt;/em&gt;LEA.&lt;/p&gt;&lt;p&gt;Wartner, U. G., Grossman, K., Fremmer-Bombik, I., &amp;amp; Guess, G. L. (1994). Attachment patterns in south Germany. &lt;em&gt;Child Development, 65&lt;/em&gt;, 1014-27.&lt;/p&gt;&lt;p&gt;Wolff, M. S., &amp;amp; Ijzendoorn, M. H. (1997). &lt;a target="_blank" href="https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Marinus_Van_IJzendoorn/publication/13914401_Sensitivity_and_Attachment_A_Meta-Analysis_on_Parental_Antecedents_of_Infant_Attachment/links/00b7d5248672a34969000000.pdf"&gt;Sensitivity and attachment: A meta‐analysis on parental antecedents of infant attachment&lt;/a&gt;. &lt;em&gt;Child development, 68(4)&lt;/em&gt;, 571-591.&lt;/p&gt;&lt;p style="margin-top:20px"&gt;&lt;/p&gt;&lt;div class="panel panel-primary"&gt;&lt;div class="panel-heading"&gt;Further Information&lt;/div&gt;&lt;div class="list-group"&gt;&lt;a target="_blank" href="https://www.simplypsychology.org/attachment-styles.html" class="list-group-item"&gt;Attachment Styles&lt;/a&gt; &lt;a target="_blank" href="https://www.simplypsychology.org/a-level-attachment.html" class="list-group-item"&gt;A-level Psychology Attachment Revision Notes&lt;/a&gt; &lt;a target="_blank" href="https://www.simplypsychology.org/culture-attachment.html" class="list-group-item"&gt;Culture Differences in Attachment&lt;/a&gt; &lt;a target="_blank" href="https://www.simplypsychology.org/bowlby.html" class="list-group-item"&gt;John Bowlby&lt;/a&gt; &lt;a target="_blank" href="https://thepsychologist.bps.org.uk/volume-30/january-2017/overrated-predictive-power-attachment" class="list-group-item"&gt;BPS Article- Overrated: The predictive power of attachment&lt;/a&gt; &lt;a target="_blank" href="http://www.psychology.sunysb.edu/attachment/measures/content/ss_scoring.pdf" class="list-group-item"&gt;Scoring for the Strange Situation &lt;span class="glyphicon glyphicon-download-alt"&gt;&lt;/span&gt;&lt;/a&gt;&lt;a target="_blank" href="https://www.simplypsychology.org/The%20Effects%20of%20Childcare%20on%20Social%20Development.pdf" class="list-group-item"&gt;The Effects of Childcare on Social Development&lt;/a&gt; &lt;a target="_blank" href="http://www.psychology.sunysb.edu/attachment/online/attach_depend.pdf" class="list-group-item"&gt;A theoretical review of the infant-mother relationship &lt;span class="glyphicon glyphicon-download-alt"&gt;&lt;/span&gt;&lt;/a&gt;&lt;a target="_blank" href="https://www.simplypsychology.org/Attachment%20-%20Bowlby%20%26%20Ainsworth.pdf" class="list-group-item"&gt;The Origins of Attachment Theory: Bowlby &amp;amp; Ainsworth &lt;span class="glyphicon glyphicon-download-alt"&gt;&lt;/span&gt;&lt;/a&gt;&lt;a target="_blank" href="https://www.simplypsychology.org/Ainsworth.ppt" class="list-group-item"&gt;Ainsworth Strange Situation &lt;span class="glyphicon glyphicon-download-alt"&gt;&lt;/span&gt;&lt;/a&gt;&lt;a target="_blank" href="https://openaccess.leidenuniv.nl/bitstream/handle/1887/11634/7_702_111.pdf" class="list-group-item"&gt;Cross-cultural Patterns of Attachment: A Meta-Analysis of the Strange Situation &lt;span class="glyphicon glyphicon-download-alt"&gt;&lt;/span&gt;&lt;/a&gt;&lt;a target="_blank" href="https://digest.bps.org.uk/2019/05/09/first-study-to-investigate-how-attachment-style-changes-through-multiple-decades-of-life/" class="list-group-item"&gt;How Attachment Style Changes Through Multiple Decades Of Life&lt;/a&gt; &lt;a target="_blank" href="https://www.bbc.co.uk/iplayer/console/p00f8n6q" class="list-group-item"&gt;Mary Ainsworth &lt;span class="glyphicon glyphicon-headphones"&gt;&lt;/span&gt;&lt;/a&gt;&lt;/div&gt;&lt;span class="ezoic-autoinsert-ad ezoic-long_content"&gt;&lt;/span&gt;&lt;/div&gt;&lt;p style="margin-top:25px"&gt;&lt;/p&gt;&lt;div class="well well-lg"&gt;&lt;button type="button" class="btn btn-default btn-lg"&gt;&lt;span class="glyphicon glyphicon-download-alt"&gt;&lt;/span&gt; &lt;a target="_blank" href="https://www.simplypsychology.org/simplypsychology.org-mary-ainsworth.pdf" rel="alternate" type="application/pdf" onclick="_gaq.push(['_trackEvent','Download-Bottom','Ainsworth-PDF-Bottom']);"&gt;&lt;/a&gt;&lt;/button&gt;&lt;a target="_blank" href="https://www.simplypsychology.org/simplypsychology.org-mary-ainsworth.pdf" rel="alternate" type="application/pdf" onclick="_gaq.push(['_trackEvent','Download-Bottom','Ainsworth-PDF-Bottom']);"&gt; &amp;nbsp;Download this article as a PDF&lt;/a&gt;&lt;/div&gt;&lt;p style="margin-top:20px"&gt;&lt;/p&gt;&lt;div class="panel panel-default"&gt;&lt;div class="panel-body"&gt;&lt;p&gt;&lt;/p&gt;&lt;h4 class="hidden-xs"&gt;&lt;strong&gt;How to reference this article:&lt;/strong&gt;&lt;/h4&gt;&lt;p&gt;&lt;/p&gt;&lt;p&gt;&lt;/p&gt;&lt;h5 class="visible-xs"&gt;&lt;strong&gt;How to reference this article:&lt;/strong&gt;&lt;/h5&gt;&lt;p&gt;&lt;/p&gt;&lt;p class="text-break"&gt;&lt;small&gt;McLeod, S. A. (2018, August 05). &lt;em&gt;Mary ainsworth&lt;/em&gt;. Simply Psychology. www.simplypsychology.org/mary-ainsworth.html&lt;/small&gt;&lt;/p&gt;&lt;/div&gt;&lt;/div&gt;&lt;p&gt;&lt;/p&gt;&lt;div class="panel panel-default"&gt;&lt;div class="panel-body" style="background-color:#f2f2f2"&gt;&lt;div align="center"&gt;&lt;p&gt;&lt;script&gt;var domainroot="www.simplypsychology.org" function Gsitesearch(curobj){curobj.q.value="site:"+domainroot+" "+curobj.qfront.value}</p><form class="search navbar-form navbar-center" action="https://www.google.com/search" role="search" onsubmit="Gsitesearch(this)" onclick="_gaq.push(['_trackEvent','Search','Search-bottom']);"><div class="input-group"><input name="q" type="hidden"> <input class="form-control" name="qfront" type="search" required placeholder="Google Site Search" maxlength="300"> <span class="input-group-btn"><button class="search-button btn btn-primary"> <span class="glyphicon glyphicon-search"></span></button></span></div></form><p></p><p style="margin-top:20px"></p><a target="_blank" href="https://www.simplypsychology.org/">Home</a> <span class="text-separator">|</span> <a target="_blank" href="https://www.simplypsychology.org/about.html">About Us</a> <span class="text-separator">|</span> <a target="_blank" href="https://www.simplypsychology.org/privacy.html">Privacy Policy</a> <span class="text-separator">|</span> <a target="_blank" href="https://simplypsychology.org/direct-sales.html">Advertise</a> <span class="text-separator">|</span> <a target="_blank" href="https://www.simplypsychology.org/about.html">Contact Us</a><p style="margin-top:15px"></p><div align="center"><a target="_blank" href="https://www.simplypsychology.org/mary-ainsworth.html#" onclick="_gaq.push(['_trackEvent','Back-to-top','Back-to-top']);" button="" type="button" class="btn btn-default"><span class="glyphicon glyphicon-menu-up" aria-hidden="true"></span><br>Back to top</a></div><p class="text-muted" style="margin-top:15px">Simply Psychology's content is for informational and educational purposes only. Our website is not intended to be a substitute for professional medical advice, diagnosis, or treatment.</p><p class="text-muted">© Simply Scholar Ltd - All rights reserved</p></div></div></div><p></p><span id="ezoic-pub-ad-placeholder-199"></span><span class="ezoic-ad ezoic-at-0 medrectangle-1 medrectangle-1199 adtester-container adtester-container-199" data-ez-name="simplypsychology_org-medrectangle-1"><span id="div-gpt-ad-simplypsychology_org-medrectangle-1-0" ezaw="250" ezah="250" style="position:relative;z-index:0;display:inline-block;padding:0;width:100%;max-width:1200px;margin-left:auto!important;margin-right:auto!important;min-height:400px;min-width:580px" class="ezoic-ad"><script data-ezscrex="false" data-cfasync="false" style="display:none">if(typeof ez_ad_units!='undefined'){ez_ad_units.push([[250,250],'simplypsychology_org-medrectangle-1','ezslot_22',199,'0','0'])};__ez_fad_position('div-gpt-ad-simplypsychology_org-medrectangle-1-0');

What research method does the Strange Situation use?

What researcher conducted the Strange Situation experiment?

The American-Canadian psychologist Mary Ainsworth (1913-1999) developed the Strange Situation Procedure (SSP) to measure mother-child attachment and attachment theorists have used it ever since.

What is Strange Situation procedure in psychology?

an experimental technique used to assess quality of attachment in infants and young children (up to the age of 2). The procedure subjects the child to increasing amounts of stress induced by a strange setting, the entrance of an unfamiliar person, and two brief separations from the parent.

What is the general research conclusion regarding the Strange Situation?

What was the conclusion of the Strange Situation study? There appears to be an association between the mother's behaviour and the child's attachment type. Negative points of the Strange Situation.